Sunday, July 4, 2021

false confessions; (Part Five): Why do people falsely confess? Criminal lawyer Marissa Boyers Bluestine's candid explanation: "Without physical coercion, police have fallen back to rely upon interrogation tactics that lean heavily on psychological measures, and that courts still allow despite empirical research demonstrating their coercive effect. Police are trained as “human lie detectors” in order to distinguish truthful witnesses from deceptive suspects. There is no evidence that such training actually works, but it nonetheless controls how police conduct interrogations. Following this dubious training, officers engage in questioning techniques aimed not at getting information from the individual about a crime under investigation, but at analyzing the person in front of them to discern their inner motivations. If police decide someone is lying or is refusing to admit to a crime the police believe they committed, the point of interrogation has little to do with finding the truth, and everything to do with securing a confession. Unlike in other parts of the world, police in the United States are allowed to lie to suspects during an interrogation. Often police use a “false evidence ploy” to increase the pressure on a suspect toward confessing—whether they committed the crime or not. This particular tactic has been cited by leading false confession experts as a main driver behind innocent people “confessing” to crimes they never committed."


PUBLISHER'S NOTE: This Blog is interested in false confessions because of the disturbing number of exonerations in the USA, Canada and multiple other jurisdictions throughout the world, where, in the absence of incriminating forensic evidence the conviction is based on self-incrimination – and because of the growing body of  scientific research showing how vulnerable suspects (especially young suspects)  are to widely used interrogation methods  such as  the notorious ‘Reid Technique.’ As  all too many of this Blog's post have shown, I also recognize that pressure for false confessions can take many forms, up to and including inducement. deception (read ‘outright lies’) physical violence,  and even physical and mental torture.

Harold Levy: Publisher; The Charles Smith Blog:

---------------------------------------------------------------------------

QUOTE OF THE DAY:

“I am going to say it folks: people who have no idea, who weren’t there, who weren’t involved, do not confess, especially to murder. It goes against everything you have ever known and all your common sense. No one confesses to a murder that they did not commit; no one.” 

Prosecutor’s closing argument, Commonwealth v. Willie Veasy, p. 108.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

PASSAGE OF THE DAY: "As a criminal defense lawyer, I heard many prosecutors make that same argument to juries. And it’s kind of hard to argue with that logic. Yet, we know that people do, in fact, confess to crimes they never committed. Demonstrably false confessions plague wrongful convictions. Since the National Registry of Exonerations began tracking exonerations, consistently 20 percent of all exonerations have involved someone who “confessed” to a crime they didn’t commit. How can that be? And why aren’t false confessions discovered before a wrongful conviction happens?

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

COMMENTARY: "False Confessions Explained," by Marissa Boyers Bluestine, published by 'The Appeal' on June 7, 2021. "Marissa Boyers Bluestine is an Assistant Director of the Quattrone Center for the Fair Administration of Justice at the University of Pennsylvania Law School." Some bad news: 'The Appeal' - an excellent, informative criminal justice publication - as explained  at the link below regrettably has been wound up effective June 30. Efforts are being made to revive it. I will follow developments. HL)


----------------------------------------------------------------------------------

GIST: "In Theory, Coerced Confessions Cannot Be Used As Evidence": The only mechanism to prevent the state from entering a statement or confession is by moving to “suppress” the statement or any testimony about it.A motion to suppress evidence is a pretrial motion where the defense asks the court to prevent the prosecution from presenting evidence secured in violation of the Constitution. Most exonerees who falsely confessed sought to keep the government from using their false statement at trial but were denied.

In the case of a statement, defense lawyers typically allege violations of the Fifth Amendment (the right against coerced self-incrimination) or of the due process clause of the 14th Amendment. Motions under the Fifth Amendment ask the court to find that the statement was obtained without the person’s knowing, intelligent, and voluntary waiver of his rights to remain silent and not incriminate himself. Whether a statement is given involuntarily turns on whether the individual’s will has been “overcome” and the confession was “obtained by compulsion.” As the Supreme Court observed over one hundred years ago, “[i]n short, the true test of admissibility is that the confession is made freely, voluntarily, and without compulsion or inducement of any sort.” 

Whether a statement is made voluntarily generally hinges on two questions: (1) did the suspect receive his required Miranda warnings; and (2) was there coercion—both as to the decision to waive the right to remain silent and speak with police, and the confession the person ultimately provided? The answer to the first question is usually almost pro forma: officers typically produce a small card or sheet where the suspect answered the Mirandarequirements. The burden then falls on the interrogated person to prove that police obtained that waiver improperly. Once a court determines someone voluntarily waived his right against self-incrimination, the court turns to the due process question: did police coerce the person into providing the statement?

Under this standard, courts have effectively outlawed various police practices in the interrogation room over the past century, most broadly the use of physical force to coerce a suspect into “confessing.” Gruesome depictions of physical abuse are legion: whippings, Brown v. Mississippi, 297 U.S. 278 (1936). sleep deprivation, physical beatings, cigarette burns, and even thinly veiled threats of lynching have all been used by police to obtain confessions. Aside from physical coercion, courts have recognized limited types of mental coercion that could invalidate a confession such as threats of the death penalty, outright promises of leniency, or threats of physical harm.  

In Practice, Police Coercion Happens All The Time:

To be sure, Miranda warnings and these other doctrinal rules are insufficient to prevent false confessions. Allegations of physical coercion occur less frequently, but false confessions continue. If not using physical force, and not outright threatening or making blatant false promises, how are police getting innocent people to confess to crimes they did not commit? And why are courts seemingly powerless to stop it from happening?

Without physical coercion, police have fallen back to rely upon interrogation tactics that lean heavily on psychological measures, and that courts still allow despite empirical research demonstrating their coercive effect. Police are trained as “human lie detectors” in order to distinguish truthful witnesses from deceptive suspects.  There is no evidence that such training actually works, but it nonetheless controls how police conduct interrogations. Following this dubious training, officers engage in questioning techniques aimed not at getting information from the individual about a crime under investigation, but at analyzing the person in front of them to discern their inner motivations. If police decide someone is lying or is refusing to admit to a crime the police believe they committed, the point of interrogation has little to do with finding the truth, and everything to do with securing a confession.

Unlike in other parts of the world, police in the United States are allowed to lie to suspects during an interrogation.  Often police use a “false evidence ploy” to increase the pressure on a suspect toward confessing—whether they committed the crime or not. This particular tactic has been cited by leading false confession experts as a main driver behind innocent people “confessing” to crimes they never committed.  

When police interrogated Veasy, they told him that multiple witnesses identified him as being the shooter. A lie. Veasy didn’t know that and thought his best option was to “tell them what they wanted to hear.” Likewise, Minneapolis police told 16-year-old Myon Burrell his own mother admitted he was “capable” of shooting someone. That was a lie. But it shook Myon enough to have him question what was happening around him and he was led into a false confession. When 17-year-old Marty Tankleff was interrogated about his parents’ murder in New York, police told him his father used his dying breaths to accuse him of the crime. Utterly fabricated. Courts did not find any of these tactics to be coercive, and therefore upheld them as constitutional.

Another major factor in false confessions is “contamination.” Showing crime scene photos or sharing other evidence is a typical way police contaminate a person’s story, prompting them to incorporate facts about a crime of which they otherwise have no knowledge.  It is how innocent people with no knowledge of a crime learn what happened: police can unintentionally (or intentionally) leak information to the suspect when trying to convince them of the strength of the “evidence” against them.

As shown in the widely viewed Netflix documentary “Making a Murderer,” this is precisely what the police did to 16-year-old Brendan Dassey, who the police suspected of murder. When officers interrogated Brendan (after he waived his Miranda rights) about the murder of a photographer on his uncle’s property, they became visibly frustrated that Brendan would not admit to shooting the victim in the head—a fact unknown outside the investigation. Finally, the detective just asked, “Who shot her in the head?” to which Brendan replied, “he did” (meaning his uncle Steve Avery). When asked why he didn’t mention that earlier, Brendan said he “couldn’t think of it.”[

Another tactic linked to false confessions is minimizing the moral depravity of a criminal act to make it more “acceptable” for someone to confess. The tactic is taught as posing an “alternative question.” The interrogator is trained to ask a question with two possibilities so that either answer is incriminating. For instance, the interrogator may ask, “Did you rape her in the spur of the moment, or did you plan it beforehand?” (the option “I didn’t rape anyone” is not presented). The first option carries a lower moral culpability and researchers believe it also carries an implied promise of leniency.  Indeed, in the notorious wrongful conviction involving five innocent boys convicted of raping a woman in Central Park in the 1980s—known collectively as the Exonerated Five— all of the boys who “confessed” believed they would be allowed to go home for their cooperation.

Courts, however, do not recognize these techniques as “coercive.” Even with experts testifying about the coerciveness of these techniques, courts consistently refuse to keep these statements from the jury.Other questions, such as whether the statement has internal reliability, are left for the jury to work out. Had reliability been a precursor to admissibility in  Veasy’s case, the jury would not have had his “confession” to consider and he would likely have been spared 28 years of wrongful imprisonment.

What Can Be Done?

Efforts to reform police interrogation tactics are increasing. A predominant suggestion is for police to be required to audio and video record the entire interaction with someone. According to the Innocence Project, more than 25 states and the District of Columbia require recordings of custodial interrogations. Some of these requirements, like Alaska’s, are by court action, but most are through legislation. Even some federal agencies—the FBI, the Drug Enforcement Agency (DEA), and the Bureau of Alcohol, Tobacco, Firearms and Explosives (ATF)—are required to record all custodial interrogations of individuals suspected of any federal crime. 

New York is considering legislation that would go further, and prohibit police from “knowingly communicating false facts about evidence” to someone during an interrogation; in other words, no more lying.  Some courts have also started to push back on police deception. In 2019, the Hawaii Supreme Court ruled that lying to people about their results from a polygraph test is inherently coercive, and that any statements given in response must be excluded from evidence. Lying about lie detector tests—telling people that they failed when in fact they passed—is a police tactic to trick people into thinking the evidence against them is stronger than it is. It can even make innocent people question their own certainty and consider the possibility that they may be guilty. It is, as researcher Saul Kassin writes, why innocence puts innocents at risk for falsely confessing. 

“Extensive scientific literature and numerous documented cases have demonstrated the coercive nature of falsified polygraph test results,” the Hawaii Supreme Court wrote. “They can change a suspect’s beliefs, pressure a suspect to confess, and even cause the suspect to believe they committed the crime when they did not.” That is, the court said, “false polygraph results may psychologically prime an innocent suspect to make a confession.”

Other courts could follow this lead, and recognize how police deception of all kinds is often coercive and therefore unconstitutional.

And the police themselves are increasingly looking for ways to interrogate that avoid coercion and are geared toward good-faith fact-finding over securing confessions. In response to the state terrorism of “enhanced interrogation” techniques and torture by the George W. Bush Administration, President Obama authorized the Department of Defense to fund research in psychology and the behavioral sciences to understand what works and what doesn’t in interrogations. This effort, known as the High Value Detainee Interrogation Group (HIG), has produced hundreds of studies examining the science of interrogations. The Los Angeles Police Department has trained some of its officers in tactics developed by HIG-funded research centered on cognitive interviewing. Similarly, a detective in Tempe, Arizona has collaborated with researchers to develop a completely science-based interview curriculum that moves away from traditional confrontational and accusatory methods and toward non-coercive information-gathering techniques supported by field validation studies. But with over 6,000 law enforcement agencies in the U.S., there is a lot of work left to do.

Even moderate reforms, such as videotaping the interrogation, would have allowed Veasy’s jury to see what happened in the interrogation room and judge for themselves whether it was a true confession. Had police engaged in an investigative interview rather than a coercive one, he wouldn’t have been in that defendant’s chair at all.

False confessions are a problem of enormous consequence; ending them is an ethical and moral imperative. Reducing the risk of false confession and preventing injustices begin with the police to be sure, but responsibility for truly stopping them lies with all the stakeholders in the criminal legal system."

The entire commentary can be read at:

https://theappeal.org/the-lab/explainers/false-confessions-explained/

PUBLISHER'S NOTE: I am monitoring this case/issue. Keep your eye on the Charles Smith Blog for reports on developments. The Toronto Star, my previous employer for more than twenty incredible years, has put considerable effort into exposing the harm caused by Dr. Charles Smith and his protectors - and into pushing for reform of Ontario's forensic pediatric pathology system. The Star has a "topic"  section which focuses on recent stories related to Dr. Charles Smith. It can be found at: http://www.thestar.com/topic/charlessmith. Information on "The Charles Smith Blog Award"- and its nomination process - can be found at: http://smithforensic.blogspot.com/2011/05/charles-smith-blog-award-nominations.html Please send any comments or information on other cases and issues of interest to the readers of this blog to: hlevy15@gmail.com.  Harold Levy: Publisher: The Charles Smith Blog;
-----------------------------------------------------------------
FINAL WORD:  (Applicable to all of our wrongful conviction cases):  "Whenever there is a wrongful conviction, it exposes errors in our criminal legal system, and we hope that this case — and lessons from it — can prevent future injustices."
Lawyer Radha Natarajan:
Executive Director: New England Innocence Project;
—————————————————————————————————
FINAL, FINAL WORD: "Since its inception, the Innocence Project has pushed the criminal legal system to confront and correct the laws and policies that cause and contribute to wrongful convictions.   They never shied away from the hard cases — the ones involving eyewitness identifications, confessions, and bite marks. Instead, in the course of presenting scientific evidence of innocence, they’ve exposed the unreliability of evidence that was, for centuries, deemed untouchable." So true!
Christina Swarns: Executive Director: The Innocence Project.